Running head: DIAGNOSING HOMESICKNESS

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Diagnosing Camper Homesickness: Two Systems

Renee Bader

Anderson University

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Introduction

 

            Summer camp is designed to be a positive, fun, and safe experience for campers. However, homesickness is common and expected, and can interfere with the camper’s experience, group dynamics, and increase the counselor’s stress level. Home sickness has been defined as “longing for home and family while absent from them” (Homesickness, 2009). Thurber, Sigman, Weisz, and Schmidt (1999) discuss homesickness as “distress or impairment” before or after the separation, and also it is marked by “longing and preoccupying thoughts of home and attachment objects.”

            Understanding and diagnosing homesickness is very important to any camp staff. The well-being of the camper is very important, as well as the ability of other campers to enjoy their time and have proper supervision and equal counselor attention. It also is important to understand in the emotional drain, frustration, or excess stress it can cause for counselors. Homesickness diagnoses are made when deciding how to best treat the camper, from extra support, to the process of calling parents and even sending the camper home early.

            The first Diagnostic System is based on the emotional responses of the camper. It is broken into three categories: mild, moderate, and severe, based on the camper’s level of four different aspects of emotional responses common in homesick individuals. The first variable is crying, and the situations change and amount increase through the levels of the diagnosis. The second measured emotion is anger, ranging from slightly inappropriate anger responses to definite overreactions in inappropriate situations. Fear reaction is the third criteria, progressing similarly to anger responses. Finally, general emotion displays, intended to measure appropriateness, intensity, and mobility of affect. The diagnosis is to be made based on what category has the most correct criterion, with at least two of the criteria of the level being appropriate. Appendix A is the full emotional diagnostic system. I estimate that the validity and reliability estimates will lower in this system, as compared to the behavioral system. Emotional criterion is difficult to measure, and I think this may make this system less reliable. Also, the test diagnosticians lack of familiarity with camp, or even with children, could lower the overall success of both systems.

            The second diagnostic system is based on behavioral indications of homesickness. The first behavior in the system is hesitation or lack of participation in activities, ranging from high to low risk actives; as the intensity of homesickness increases, participation will decrease for not only high risk but also low risk activities. The second behavioral criteria has to do with physical illness, from reporting and showing minor discomfort (headache or stomachache) to illness that keeps the camper from participating regularly in camp. The next aspect is showing shy or reserved behavior in relating to other campers; this ranges from mild hesitation to refusal to interact with others. Next is a criterion that applies only to moderately and severely homesick campers; this is asking to call or return home or even lying about returning home early. The diagnosis in this system is to be made by selecting the level that has the most correct descriptions, with at least two of the behavioral aspects matching the case. The full behavioral system can be found in Appendix B. I estimate that reliability and validity will be near 90 percent for the behavioral system. The criteria in this system are more easily measureable and observable than the other, thus I expect higher reliability and validity. Lack of familiarly with camp procedures and normal camper reactions in the test-diagnosticians is one reason I do not expect perfect reliability and validity.

 

 

 

Method

Participants

            Ten students were selected randomly from an Undergraduate Abnormal Psychology class in a small, private, Midwest University to serve as test- diagnosticians for the two systems. Reverend Bill Graham, ordained elder in the United Methodist Church, served as the expert for this study. He has served as the chair for the Board of Camp and Retreat Ministries, as well as  13 summers as Director of Camp Asbury, where one of his main duties is addressing homesick campers, contacting families, and making decisions about early dismissal of campers.

Procedure

            Four cases of homesick campers (see Appendix C) were presented to the ten test diagnosticians, along with the two diagnostic systems. These students were asked to diagnose each case on both systems, and these results were used to determine inter-judge reliability. To determine validity, the four cases were presented to Reverend Graham, without the diagnostic systems, and he was asked to rate each camper as mildly, moderately, or severely homesick (as are the categories in the two systems).

Results

Emotional

System 1

Case 1

Case 2

Case 3

Mild

0

9*

0

R=26/30

86%

 

V= 22/30

73%

Moderate

3*

1

0

Severe

7

0

10*

*indicates Expert’s Choice

 

 

Behavioral

System 2

Case 1

Case 2

Case 3

Mild

1

9*

0

R=25/30

83%

 

V= 25/30

83%

Moderate

6*

1

1

Severe

3

0

10*

*indicates Expert’s Choice

 

 

Discussion

 

Information is lost in both systems, as happens in any diagnostic system. In both the emotional and behaviors systems, information about how the camper’s homesickness is affecting the group dynamics and the counselors is lost. These are both significant questions when making decisions about treating homesick campers. The emotional system also looses information about how well a camper is coping with these emotions; a camper could be very emotional and homesick, but still able to process the emotions and function well. However, with the emotional system, one can gain an understanding of how extensive and intense the emotional impact of the homesickness is for the camper.

The behavioral system does not capture how deeply the camper is being affected; it is possible that homesickness could be highly internalized and a camper is emotionally unstable, without strong behavior cues. This system gives good information about the way a camper’s homesickness is affecting their behavior, and even gives some information about the emotional displays of the camper. After talking with my expert, I think that the emotional system also loses an important demotion, one that is significant to prognosis. Reverend Graham shared that the case that he diagnosed as severe (Case 3- Joey) was marked by anger, a male typical response, but Joey had not yet “shut down.” He shared that he felt this camper could still be helped through the week of camp, but when a camper “shuts-down” they will likely not recover or make it through the entire week of camp. This aspect is not found in either system, but it very important in the understanding of homesickness in practice.

The first system was more reliable (86%) however; it had the lowest validity (73%). While this could be attributed to many things, I think that it indicates that in efforts to be clear in distinctions, I compromised up the reality of the system. Also, it indicates that I did not fully capture well the reality of the emotions behind homesickness. In the future, I would include the “shutting down” theory as a part of the emotional system, as well as aspects of how responsive the camper is to counselor intervention. I feel these adaptations would make the system more valid. Reliability changes apply to both systems and will be addressed together.

The behavioral system two had reliability and validity both at 83 percent; this is slightly lower than I predicted (near 90 percent was my hypothesis). Interestingly, I thought that the second system would be more reliable and it was not, however, this was only by three percent (one rating). Some of this may be attributed to lack of familiarity with camp and children; for example, recognizing what kinds of crying are normal or abnormal for children, and in the camp atmosphere. Including lists, or even simply definitions, of high and low risk activities is one thing I would do in the future to help balance the lack of familiarity with camp.

Both systems were difficult because the same criterion are present for each diagnosis with only ambiguous differences in the levels. Including category breaks with specific numbers of actions would help with reliability, but would not be valid unless the situations were still taken into account. An alternative would be creating a point system where each action gets points based on severity, situation, and number of occurrences. I think this fits into the “spectrum” idea that would be helpful to validity, however, it would be more complex and not likely to have good diagnostician compliance.

In regards to diagnostician compliance, I had only one diagnostician did not follow directions and clearly mark which category the case was (mild, moderate, severe). This person did circle individual sub points of the categories. I decided that it was clear enough to use because in two cases only one category was marked. In one case, the “crying” criterion was circled in both the mild and moderate categories. I counted this as moderate, reasoning that the mild criterion for crying was marked, and indeed the diagnosis would be true, alongside the strong “moderate” crying indicator which was also marked.

I think that many improvements could be made to these systems; working to change the criteria to more valid emotions and behaviors, finding clearer ways to distinguish between the levels of diagnosis, and a clearer set of directions would be the most significant changes I would make. Despite the shortcomings, the systems do hold potential; the reliability and validity are not as strong as I would like, but they do seem to indicate that the systems somewhat captures the issue. The systems do seem to offer some meaningful indications of how a camper would be affected by homesickness, and this information would have some helpful function to diagnosing homesick campers, which is the ultimate goal of the systems.

This experience reinforced my thoughts that researchers must be humble; there is no perfect research, and others always have wisdom that should be accepted with grace. Creating a diagnostic system takes careful thought, and to capture the truth of an issue, at least in my experience, one must think about the issue from many angles (as a camper, a counselor, a parent, and a camp director). I also was struck by the way the homesickness models depression, and how responsiveness is a large part of diagnosing homesickness.

 

References

Homesickness. (2009). In Merriam-Webster Online Dictionary. Retrieved October 25, 2009, from http://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/homesickness

 

Thurber, C., Sigman, M., Weisz, J., & Schmidt, C. (1999). Homesickness in preadolescent and adolescent girls: Risk factors, behavioral correlates, and sequelae. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 28(2), 185-196. http://search.ebscohost.com


 

Appendix A

 

Emotional Diagnostic System

  Mild

·         A few tears shed at trigger times (mail, meals, bed)

·         Mild over reaction to anger-producing situations

·         New or uncharacteristic fear

·         Mild un-provoked emotional displays*

  Moderate

·         Significant crying at trigger times (mail, meals, bed)

·         Over reaction to anger-producing situations or unprovoked anger

·         Interfering fear in mild situations

·         Emotional displays* without trigger events

  Severe

·         Consistent crying at trigger times

·         Extreme anger in mild situations

·         Fear in extreme amounts or unprovoked situations

·         Constant and/or excessive emotional displays*

*Emotion displays would involve inappropriate situation, intensity, or mobility of affect (outward emotional expression)

 


 

Appendix B

 

Behavioral Diagnostic System

 

Mild

  • Hesitant to participate in high-risk activities
  • Reporting of mild physical illness (headache/stomachache)
  • Slow to build relationships or talk to others
  • Occasional, provoked crying

Moderate

  • Hesitant to participate in low-risk activities
  • Reporting or showing illness
  • Out of personality shy and reserved behavior
  • Abnormal crying and other abnormal emotional responses
  • Asking to call or return home

Severe

  • Hesitant or refusal to participate in any activities
  • Showing signs of physical illness that interferes with camp life
  • Refusal to speak or interact with others
  • Excessive crying and inappropriate emotional responses
  • Asking or demanding to call home, return home
  • Lying about parents coming or leaving camp early

 

 


 

Appendix C: Case Studies

 

Case One:

Name: Brian (male)

Entering Grade: 2

Number of Previous Summers at Camp: 0

Participation: Brian will reluctantly participate in music or art, but refuses to swim or play on the beach. Most other daily activities involve a fight with his counselors.

Emotional: Transition times are highly emotional times, he is sure to begin crying and find a counselor saying “I would like to see my mother, please.” He also will get angry when activities are far away, and sit down, crying or yelling, and refuse to go with the group without much counselor attention.

 Health: The concern is about eating. Brain isn’t eating at meal times, saying he doesn’t feel well, and sits with his head down at the table, not joining in the conversations.

 

Case Two:

 Name: Sarah (female)

Entering Grade: 5

Number of Previous Summers at Camp: 2

Participation: Sarah loves swim time, especially building sand castles with the girls in her group. During music and art, she really opens up and it’s the one time of day she will freely speak (without being directly addressed). Sarah, though not loud, has participated in all activities, except the most challenging elements on the low-ropes course.

Emotional: Sarah and cried quietly twice at dinner when she read letters from her family at dinner time.

Health: Sarah says she has a stomach ache every morning, and Wednesday was adamant enough that her counselors too her to see the nurse.

 

 

Chase Three:

Name: Joey

Entering Grade: 7

Number of Previous Summers at Camp: 0

Participation: He resists activities throughout the day, consistently lagging behind his group from the moment the lights go on in the morning. While there are some activities he will soften up to, transition times are highly emotional. He will become angry with his counselors, yelling or completely shutting down, never willingly going. He commonly starts yelling that he will run away from camp unless he is allowed to call his parents and tell them what he is being forced to do. He protested to the point of the camp allowing him to stay back from a 7 mile canoe trip.

Emotional: He spends most of the day angry, but his family wrote letters for every day, and Joey reads them after dinner, and cries for nearly twenty minutes, ignoring his fellow campers and counselors reassuring comments.

Health: Joey asks to go to the nurse after almost every meal, saying he is going to throw up, and needs to lie down.